East and West: Seeing
the world through differerent lenses.
East is East and West is West, and
the difference between them is starting to turn up even on brain scanners.
New brain research is adding
high-tech evidence to what lower-tech psychology experiments have found for
years: Culture can affect not just language and custom, but how people
experience the world at stunningly basic levels - what they see when they look
at a city street, for example, or even how they perceive a simple line in a
square.
Western culture, they have found,
conditions people to think of themselves as highly independent entities. And
when looking at scenes, Westerners tend to focus on central objects more than
on their surroundings.
In contrast, East Asian cultures
stress interdependence. When Easterners take in a scene, they tend to focus
more on the context as well as the object: the whole block, say, rather than
the BMW parked in the foreground.
To use a camera analogy, "the
Americans are more zoom and the East Asians are more panoramic," said Dr.
Denise Park of the Center for Brain Health at the University of Texas in
Dallas. "The Easterner probably sees more, and the Westerner probably sees
less, but in more detail."
In January, researchers led by Trey
Hedden and John Gabrieli at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology showed
that such deeply ingrained habits of thought affect the brains of East Asians
and Americans even as they perform simple tasks that involve estimating the
length of a line.
Hedden's experiment involved two
tasks. In one, subjects eyeballed a line simply to estimate its length - a task
that played to American strengths. In another, they estimated the line's length
relative to the size of a square - an easier task for the Asians.
Brain scanners measure levels of
neural activity by tracking blood flow. The experiment found that though there
was no difference in performance - the tasks were very easy - the level of
activity in the subjects' brains differed, suggesting different levels of
effort.
Areas linked to attention lit up
more in the Americans' brains when they worked on the task they tend to find
harder, estimating the line's size relative to the square. In Asians, too, the
attention areas lit up more during the harder task, estimating the line's
length without comparing it to the square.
Those findings, published in the
journal Psychological Science, echo more than a decade of previous experimental
research into East-West differences that are so fundamental that people tend
not to be consciously aware of them. A University of Michigan professor,
Richard Nisbett, even wrote a 2003 book about it, "The Geography of
Thought."
But brain scan data add new heft to
such findings, said Hazel Rose Markus, a psychology professor at Stanford
University who collaborated on the Gabrieli paper. Brain findings may help
people become aware of deep cultural differences that are normally "so
much part of the water that we don't see them," she said.
Such differences have turned up in
experiment after experiment. For example: In one study, researchers offered
people a choice among five pens: four red and one green. Easterners are
likelier to choose a red pen, while Westerners more often choose the green.
In an experiment measuring how well
8-year-olds could solve puzzles, American children performed best when solving
puzzles they had chosen themselves, while Asian children performed best when
solving puzzles they were told their mothers had chosen for them, Markus said.
American children brought up in an independence-minded culture felt best when
they were exercising free choice, she said; while the Asian children assumed
that their mothers had their best interests at heart.
When they are tested on details of
an underwater scene they recently viewed, Westerners tend to remember more
about the biggest fish, while Easterners remember more about the scene's
background.
"Literally, our data suggest
that people see different elements of pictures," Park said. "If
you're looking at an elephant in the jungle, the Westerner will focus on the
elephant and the Easterner is going to be more thinking about the jungle scene
that has the elephant in it."
Researchers use the terms East and
West very roughly. West tends to mean Americans and people from
independence-oriented European countries or Australia. East means East Asians -
mainly Japanese, Koreans, and Chinese in research so far - as well as much of
the rest of the world.
Researchers point out that the differences
detected by psychological experiments and brain scans are not glaring; they are
subtle but detectable trends. Also, individuals within cultures vary greatly,
and gender differences can arise as well.
The brain research promises to add
new precision to the earlier work. In January's study, Gabrieli said, the
scanning not only showed brain differences on the line-and-square task, it
allowed researchers to begin to ask how deep those differences go.
Did Easterners actually see
differently, at the level of perception, or just think differently? Based on
what parts of the brain were activated during the tasks, Gabrieli believes
everyone sees the same thing, but may filter it differently.
"Culture is not changing how
you see the world, but rather how you think and interpret."
And that could be good news:
"If it changed how you saw the world, it would make the barrier higher for
people to agree on what they are seeing and talk with each other," he
said. "If it's in the thinking stage, even though our work suggests it's
harder work to see things from a different perspective, it's much more within
your reach."
The older people get, it seems, the
more pronounced those cultural differences become, as if the older you are,
"the more you're steeped in your own cultural mode of processing,"
Park said. But that does not mean such habits are immutable. Some initial
psychological studies suggest that when an Easterner goes West or vice versa,
habits of thought and perception quickly begin to change.
So beyond perhaps helping to defuse
tensions a bit between cross-cultural roommates or spouses, does East-West
brain research have applications for the real world?
It could have implications for, say,
Western mental health care workers trying to help Easterners. On a broader scale,
researchers say, it might be useful in business schools for students preparing
to work in East-West trade, to help clarify culture gaps.
"Understanding cultural
differences in the mind is really important as the world globalizes," Park
said. "There can be a lot of breakdowns in communication."
Δεν υπάρχουν σχόλια:
Δημοσίευση σχολίου
Σημείωση: Μόνο ένα μέλος αυτού του ιστολογίου μπορεί να αναρτήσει σχόλιο.